Please wait a minute...
金融研究  2022, Vol. 510 Issue (12): 130-148    
  本期目录 | 过刊浏览 | 高级检索 |
框架效应与贷款决策:一项金融素养的实验研究
陈泽阳, 刘玉珍, 孟涓涓
中国人民大学劳动人事学院,北京 100872;
北京大学光华管理学院 北京 100871
Framing Effect and Loan Take-up: An Experimental Study on Financial Literacy
CHEN Zeyang, LIU Yu-Jane, MENG Juanjuan
School of Labor and Human Resources, Renmin University of China;
Guanghua School of Management, Peking University
下载:  PDF (897KB) 
输出:  BibTeX | EndNote (RIS)      
摘要 过度借贷是一个常见的非理性决策。近年来,网络借贷市场的激烈竞争降低了申请贷款门槛,导致金融素养较低的人群开始参与网络借贷,进一步加剧了过度借贷现象。本文采用实验室实验方法,给被试者提供本质上完全等价的贷款产品,同时外生地变化贷款成本形式(绝对数值形式的利息相比于百分数形式的利率、单期利率相比于多期复利),研究贷款成本的展示形式对贷款意愿是否存在影响。结果表明,相比于月利率,展示月利息使得被试者的贷款接受率显著上升21.3个百分点;相比于年化复利,展示月利率使得被试者的贷款接受率显著上升7.91个百分点。这两种框架效应可分别由金融素养的知识层面和思维层面所解释。本文对网络小额贷款场景下的过度借贷提出了一种新的解释——贷款成本的框架效应,并采用实验经济学方法对此提供了可靠的证据。本文研究结论提示,加强金融教育和制定相关法规双措并举,有助于人们做出更审慎的贷款决策。
服务
把本文推荐给朋友
加入引用管理器
E-mail Alert
RSS
作者相关文章
陈泽阳
刘玉珍
孟涓涓
关键词:  金融素养  过度借贷  框架效应  贷款决策    
Summary:  Over-borrowing is a common irrational decision. In recent years, fierce competition in the online loan lending market has lowered the threshold for applying for loans, leading people with low financial literacy to start participating in online borrowing, further exacerbating the phenomenon of over-borrowing.
This article examines a behavioral bias strongly associated with financial literacy—the framing effect. The framing effect can cause the market to have an incentive to manipulate the way information is presented, which implicitly manipulates consumers' decision-making. Therefore, understanding the framing effect in loan decision-making can help policy makers make more specific regulations on the displaying form of loan content and protect the rights and interests of financial consumers more effectively.
This paper uses a laboratory experiment which provides subjects with economically equivalent loan products, and at the same time exogenously change the form of loan costs to study whether the displaying form of loan costs affects loan take-up decisions. Existing literature generally designs loan decisions as intertemporal risk decisions in the laboratory (Giné et al., 2010; Wu Zuguang et al., 2012; Baland et al., 2017). On this basis, we added conditions such as loan cost, consumption value, future income, and overdue fine to simulate the characteristics of consumption loans. This experiment adopts a between-subject experimental design, and the subjects randomly enter into one of Group A (monthly interest amount), Group B (monthly interest rate), and Group C (annual compound interest rate). The information content of the three experimental groups is economically equivalent, and the only difference lies in the displaying form of the "loan cost". Taking the monthly interest rate of 0.5% as an example, Group A sees "the monthly interest rate is 0.5%", Group B sees "the monthly interest amount is 2.5 experimental coins (assuming the loan amount is 500 experimental coins)", Group C sees "the annual compound interest rate is 6.17%".
Holding other variables constant, the loan take-up rate in Group B (monthly interest amount) is 21.30 percentage points higher than that of Group A (monthly interest rate) (which is 21.30/43.42=49.06% higher than that of Group A), and it is statistically significant. Holding other variables constant, the loan take-up rate in group C (annualized compound interest) is 7.91 percentage points lower than Group A (monthly interest rate) on average (which is 7.91/43.42=18.22% lower than that of Group A), and it is also statistically significant.
This paper further explores the channels of the two framing effects and finds that they result from different channels of financial literacy. In a broad sense, financial literacy includes not only people's mastery of financial knowledge, but also people's ability to process economic information and make wise financial decisions (Lusardi and Mitchell, 2014; Liu, 2018). After the loan take-up decisions, we test the financial literacy of the subjects. More than 90% of the subjects correctly answer the questions about the conversion of interest rates and interest amount. We also test the subjects' calculating ability to proxy the heuristics in financial decision-making. Subjects with weaker calculating ability are more prone to the framing effect of interest amount and interest rate. The financial literacy quiz also finds that 71.92% of the subjects have the bias of underestimating exponential growth. The stronger the exponential growth bias, the more prone to the framing effect of single-period interest rate and multi-period compound interest. Therefore, the framing effect of interest amount and interest rates can be explained by the heuristics of financial literacy, while the framing effect of single-period interest rate and multi-period compound interest can be explained by lacking financial knowledge.
This paper proposes a new explanation for over-borrowing in the context of online consumption loan-the framing effect of loan costs, and provides clean and reliable evidence for this using the method of experimental economics. Secondly, this paper divides financial literacy into knowledge and heuristics, and distinguishes the different causes of the two framing effects.
This paper suggests that we need the combination of financial education and financial policy to manage the over-borrowing problem in the Internet age. To alleviate the framing effect of single-period interest rate and multi-period compound interest, government can provide more financial education on the concept of compound interest to the public. To alleviate the framing effect of interest amount in absolute values and interest rate in percentage points, government can directly make clear regulations on the displaying form of loan information, such as adjusting the displaying form of loan costs from single-period interest amount to single-period interest rate, or further adjusting to multi-period compound interest rate.
Keywords:  Financial Literacy    Over-borrowing    Framing Effect    Loan Take-up Decision
JEL分类号:  C91   D03   D14  
基金资助: * 刘玉珍感谢国家自然科学基金资助(项目号72172004)。孟涓涓感谢国家自然科学基金资助(项目号72225002,71822301)。
作者简介:  陈泽阳,经济学博士,师资博士后,中国人民大学劳动人事学院,E-mail:chenzeyang@ruc.edu.cn.
刘玉珍,管理学博士,教授,北京大学光华管理学院,E-mail:yjliu@gsm.pku.edu.cn.
孟涓涓,经济学博士,教授,北京大学光华管理学院,E-mail:jumeng@gsm.pku.edu.cn.
引用本文:    
陈泽阳, 刘玉珍, 孟涓涓. 框架效应与贷款决策:一项金融素养的实验研究[J]. 金融研究, 2022, 510(12): 130-148.
CHEN Zeyang, LIU Yu-Jane, MENG Juanjuan. Framing Effect and Loan Take-up: An Experimental Study on Financial Literacy. Journal of Financial Research, 2022, 510(12): 130-148.
链接本文:  
http://www.jryj.org.cn/CN/  或          http://www.jryj.org.cn/CN/Y2022/V510/I12/130
[1] 封思贤和那晋领,2020,《P2P借款人的定价偏差与被动违约风险——基于‘人人贷’数据的分析》,《金融研究》第3期,第134~151页。
[2] 高明和刘玉珍,2013,《跨国家庭金融比较:理论与政策意涵》,《经济研究》第2期,第134~149页。
[3] 江嘉骏、刘玉珍和陈康,2020,《移动互联网是否带来行为偏误——来自网络借贷市场的新证据》,《经济研究》第6期,第39~55页。
[4] 刘国强,2018,《我国消费者金融素养现状研究——基于2017年消费者金融素养问卷调查》,《金融研究》第3期,第1~20页。
[5] 刘玉珍、张峥、徐信忠和张金华,2010,《基金投资者的框架效应》,《管理世界》第2期,第25~37页。
[6] 路晓蒙、李阳、甘犁和王香,2017,《中国家庭金融投资组合的风险——过于保守还是过于冒进?》,《管理世界》第12期,第92~108页。
[7] 秦芳、王文春和何金财,2016,《金融知识对商业保险参与的影响——来自中国家庭金融调查(CHFS)数据的实证分析》,《金融研究》第10期,第143~158页。
[8] 宋全云、吴雨和尹志超,2017,《金融知识视角下的家庭信贷行为研究》,《金融研究》第6期,第95~110页。
[9] 王正位、邓颖惠和廖理,2016,《知识改变命运:金融知识与微观收入流动性》,《金融研究》第12期,第111-127页。
[10] 吴世农和吴超鹏,2005,《盈余信息度量,市场反应与投资者框架依赖偏差分析》,《经济研究》第2期,第54~62页。
[11] 吴卫星、吴锟和王琎,2018,《金融素养与家庭负债——基于中国居民家庭微观调查数据的分析》,《经济研究》第1期,第97~109页。
[12] 吴祖光、万迪昉和罗进辉,2012,《风险态度、合作行为与联保贷款契约:一个实验研究》,《金融研究》第4期,第169~182页。
[13] 杨晓兰和周业安,2017,《政府效率,社会决策机制和再分配偏好——基于中国被试者的实验经济学研究》,《管理世界》第6期,第51~62页。
[14] 姚澜和朱迅,2020,《通过额外兑付改善协调困境:一项实验研究》,《经济研究》第3期,第163~178页。
[15] 尹志超、宋全云、吴雨和彭嫦燕,2015,《金融知识、创业决策和创业动机》,《管理世界》第1期,第87~98页。
[16] 张号栋和尹志超,2016,《金融知识和中国家庭的金融排斥——基于CHFS数据的实证研究》,《金融研究》第7期,第80~95页。
[17] Agarwal, Sumit, and Bhashkar Mazumder. 2013. “Cognitive Abilities and Household Financial Decision Making.” American Economic Journal: Applied Economics 5 (1): 193~207.
[18] Andreoni, James, and Charles Sprenger. 2012. “Estimating Time Preferences from Convex Budgets.” The American Economic Review 102 (7): 3333~56.
[19] Anufriev, Mikhail, Te Bao, Angela Sutan, and Jan Tuinstra. 2019. “Fee Structure and Mutual Fund Choice: An Experiment.” Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 158 (2): 449~74.
[20] Baland, Jean-Marie, Lata Gangadharan, Pushkar Maitra, and Rohini Somanathan. 2017. “Repayment and Exclusion in a Microfinance Experiment.” Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 137: 176~90.
[21] Benartzi, Shlomo, and Richard H. Thaler. 1999. “Risk Aversion or Myopia? Choices in Repeated Gambles and Retirement Investments.” Management Science 45 (3): 364~81.
[22] Bertrand, Marianne, and Adair Morse. 2011. “Information Disclosure, Cognitive Biases, and Payday Borrowing.” The Journal of Finance 66 (6): 1865~93.
[23] Carrell, Scott, and Jonathan Zinman. 2014. “In Harm's Way? Payday Loan Access and Military Personnel Performance.” The Review of Financial Studies 27 (9): 2805~40.
[24] Carvalho, Leandro, Arna Olafsson, and Dan Silverman. 2019. “Misfortune and Mistake: The Financial Conditions and Decision-Making Ability of High-Cost Loan Borrowers.” Working Paper 26328. National Bureau of Economic Research.
[25] Chen, Shih-Fen S, Kent B Monroe, and Yung-Chien Lou. 1998. “The Effects of Framing Price Promotion Messages on Consumers' Perceptions and Purchase Intentions.” Journal of Retailing 74 (3): 353~72.
[26] Christelis, Dimitris, Tullio Jappelli, and Mario Padula. 2010. “Cognitive Abilities and Portfolio Choice.” European Economic Review 54 (1): 18~38.
[27] DelVecchio, Devon, H. Shanker Krishnan, and Daniel C. Smith. 2007. “Cents or Percent? The Effects of Promotion Framing on Price Expectations and Choice.” Journal of Marketing 71 (3): 158~70.
[28] Fehr, Ernst, and Lorenz Goette. 2007. “Do Workers Work More if Wages are high? Evidence from a Randomized Field Experiment.” American Economic Review 97 (1): 298-317.
[29] Giné, Xavier, Pamela Jakiela, Dean Karlan, and Jonathan Morduch. 2010. “Microfinance Games.” American Economic Journal: Applied Economics 2 (3): 60~95.
[30] Glaser, Markus, Zwetelina Iliewa, and Martin Weber. 2019. “Thinking about Prices Versus Thinking about Returns in Financial Markets.” The Journal of Finance 74 (6): 2997~3039.
[31] Glaser, Markus, Thomas Langer, Jens Reynders, and Martin Weber. 2007. “Framing Effects in Stock Market Forecasts: The Difference Between Asking for Prices and Asking for Returns.” Review of Finance 11 (2): 325~57.
[32] Gneezy, Uri, and Jan Potters. 1997. “An Experiment on Risk Taking and Evaluation Periods.” The Quarterly Journal of Economics 112 (2): 631~45.
[33] Heijden, Eline van der, Tobias J. Klein, Wieland Müller, and Jan Potters. 2012. “Framing Effects and Impatience: Evidence from a Large Scale Experiment.” Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 84 (2): 701~11.
[34] Holt, Charles A., and Susan K. Laury. 2002. “Risk Aversion and Incentive Effects.” The American Economic Review 92 (5): 1644~55.
[35] Huber, Christoph, and Jürgen Huber. 2019. “Scale Matters: Risk Perception, Return Expectations, and Investment Propensity under Different Scalings.” Experimental Economics 22 (1): 76~100.
[36] Kumar, Alok, and Sonya Seongyeon Lim. 2008. “How Do Decision Frames Influence the Stock Investment Choices of Individual Investors?” Management Science 54 (6): 1052~64.
[37] Laibson, David, Andrea Repetto, and Jeremy Tobacman. 2003. A Debt Puzzle. Pp. 228~66 in Knowledge, Information, and Expectations in Modern Macroeconomics: In Honor of Edmund S. Phelps, edited by Philippe Aghion, Roman Frydman, Joseph Stiglitz, and Michael Woodford. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press.
[38] Leary, Jesse, and Jialan Wang. 2016. “Liquidity Constraints and Budgeting Mistakes: Evidence from Social Security Recipients.” Working paper.
[39] Li, King King, Dan Liu, Xin Da Mai, Qiu Ju Zhang, Xiao Yu Ren, Gao Qian Xu, and Kai Zhang. 2020. “What Drive Excessive Borrowing and Under-borrowing? A Field Experiment.” SSRN Scholarly Paper ID 3795838. Rochester, NY: Social Science Research Network.
[40] Lusardi, Annamaria, and Olivia S. Mitchell. 2007. “Baby Boomer Retirement Security: The Roles of Planning, Financial Literacy, and Housing Wealth.” Journal of Monetary Economics 54 (1): 205~24.
[41] Lusardi, Annamaria, and Olivia S. Mitchell. 2014. “The Economic Importance of Financial Literacy: Theory and Evidence.” Journal of Economic Literature 52 (1): 5~44.
[42] Lusardi, Annamaria, and Carlo de Bassa Scheresberg. 2013. “Financial Literacy and High-cost Borrowing in the United States.” Working Paper 18969. National Bureau of Economic Research.
[43] Mani, Anandi, Sendhil Mullainathan, Eldar Shafir, and Jiaying Zhao. 2013. “Poverty Impedes Cognitive Function.” Science 341 (6149): 976-980.
[44] Martinez, Seung-keun, Stephan Meier, and Charles Sprenger. 2017. “Procrastination in the Field: Evidence from Tax Filing.” Working paper.
[45] Meier, Stephan, and Charles Sprenger. 2010. “Present-biased Preferences and Credit Card Borrowing.” American Economic Journal: Applied Economics 2 (1): 193~210.
[46] Melzer, Brian T. 2011. “The Real Costs of Credit Access: Evidence from the Payday Lending Market.” The Quarterly Journal of Economics 126 (1): 517-555.
[47] Melzer, Brian T. 2018. “Spillovers from Costly Credit.” The Review of Financial Studies 31 (9): 3568~94.
[48] Morgan, Donald P., Michael R. Strain, and Ihab Seblani. 2012. “How Payday Credit Access Affects Overdrafts and other outcomes.” Journal of Money, Credit and Banking 44 (2-3): 519-531.
[49] Skiba, Paige Marta, and Jeremy Tobacman. 2008. “Payday Loans, Uncertainty and Discounting: Explaining Patterns of Borrowing, Repayment, and Default.” Vanderbilt Law and Economics Research Paper, no. 08~33.
[50] Shah, Anuj K., Sendhil Mullainathan, and Eldar Shafir. 2012. “Some Consequences of Having too Little.” Science 338 (6107): 682-685.
[51] Snowberg, Erik, and Leeat Yariv. 2021. “Testing the Waters: Behavior Across Participant Pools.” American Economic Review 111 (2): 687~719.
[52] Song, Changcheng. 2020. “Financial Illiteracy and Pension Contributions: A Field Experiment on Compound Interest in China.” The Review of Financial Studies 33 (2), 916~949.
[53] Stango, Victor, and Jonathan Zinman. 2009. “Exponential Growth Bias and Household Finance.” The Journal of Finance 64 (6): 2807~49.
[54] Tversky, Amos, and Daniel Kahneman. 1981. “The Framing of Decisions and the Psychology of Choice.” Science, New Series 211 (4481): 453~58.
[55] Uribe, Martín. 2006. “On Overborrowing.” American Economic Review 96 (2): 417~21.
[1] 李政, 李鑫. 数字普惠金融与未预期风险应对:理论与实证[J]. 金融研究, 2022, 504(6): 94-114.
[2] 吴锟, 吴卫星, 王沈南. 金融教育是有效的吗?[J]. 金融研究, 2022, 509(11): 117-135.
[3] 吴卫星, 张旭阳, 吴锟. 金融素养与家庭储蓄率——基于理财规划与借贷约束的解释[J]. 金融研究, 2021, 494(8): 119-137.
[4] 郑路, 徐旻霞. 传统家庭观念抑制了城镇居民商业养老保险参与吗?———基于金融信任与金融素养视角的实证分析[J]. 金融研究, 2021, 492(6): 133-151.
[5] 刘国强. 我国消费者金融素养现状研究——基于2017年消费者金融素养问卷调查[J]. 金融研究, 2018, 453(3): 1-20.
[6] 吴锟, 吴卫星. 理财建议可以作为金融素养的替代吗?[J]. 金融研究, 2017, 446(8): 161-176.
[1] 王曦, 朱立挺, 王凯立. 我国货币政策是否关注资产价格?——基于马尔科夫区制转换BEKK多元GARCH模型[J]. 金融研究, 2017, 449(11): 1 -17 .
[2] 刘勇政, 李岩. 中国的高速铁路建设与城市经济增长[J]. 金融研究, 2017, 449(11): 18 -33 .
[3] 况伟大, 王琪琳. 房价波动、房贷规模与银行资本充足率[J]. 金融研究, 2017, 449(11): 34 -48 .
[4] 陈德球, 陈运森, 董志勇. 政策不确定性、市场竞争与资本配置[J]. 金融研究, 2017, 449(11): 65 -80 .
[5] 牟敦果, 王沛英. 中国能源价格内生性研究及货币政策选择分析[J]. 金融研究, 2017, 449(11): 81 -95 .
[6] 高铭, 江嘉骏, 陈佳, 刘玉珍. 谁说女子不如儿郎?——P2P投资行为与过度自信[J]. 金融研究, 2017, 449(11): 96 -111 .
[7] 姜军, 申丹琳, 江轩宇, 伊志宏. 债权人保护与企业创新[J]. 金融研究, 2017, 449(11): 128 -142 .
[8] 刘莎莎, 孔高文. 信息搜寻、个人投资者交易与股价联动异象——基于股票送转的研究[J]. 金融研究, 2017, 449(11): 143 -157 .
[9] 张晓宇, 徐龙炳. 限售股解禁、资本运作与股价崩盘风险[J]. 金融研究, 2017, 449(11): 158 -174 .
[10] 孙淑伟, 梁上坤, 阮刚铭, 付宇翔. 高管减持、信息压制与股价崩盘风险[J]. 金融研究, 2017, 449(11): 175 -190 .
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
版权所有 © 《金融研究》编辑部
本系统由北京玛格泰克科技发展有限公司设计开发 技术支持:support@magtech.com.cn
京ICP备11029882号-1